The daily time course of contagious and spontaneous yawning among humans. Guggisberg, A. Why do we yawn? Helt, M. Contagious yawning in autistic and typical development. Child Dev. Jackson, M. The role of attention and familiarity in face identification. Are yawns really contagious? A critique and quantification of yawn contagion. Katsuki, F. Bottom-up and top-down attention: different processes and overlapping neural systems. Neuroscientist 20, — Kawakami, K.
An eye for the I: preferential attention to the eyes of ingroup members. Krestel, H. Yawning - its anatomy, chemistry, role, and pathological considerations. Latinus, M. Discriminating male and female voices: differentiating pitch and gender.
Brain Topogr. Lehmann, H. Yawning: a homeostatic reflex and its psychological significance. Meninger Clin. Leone, A. Different yawns, different functions?
Testing social hypotheses on spontaneous yawning in Theropithecus gelada. Madsen, E. Contagious yawning in domestic dog puppies Canis lupus familiaris : the effect of ontogeny and emotional closeness on low-level imitation in dogs. Mariscal, M. Blood oxytocin concentration positively predicts contagious yawning behavior in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. Marten, K. Sound transmission and its significance for animal vocalization.
Massen, J. Auditory contagious yawning in humans: an investigation into affiliation and status effects. A thermal window for yawning in humans: yawning as a brain cooling mechanism. Why contagious yawning does not yet equate to empathy. Male yawning is more contagious than female yawning among chimpanzees Pan troglodytes. Seeing two faces together: preference formation in humans and rhesus macaques.
Michel, C. Same-race faces are perceived more holistically than other-race faces. Millen, A. Neither infants nor toddlers catch yawns from their mothers. Moyaho, A. Yawning: a cue and a signal. Heliyon 3:e Norscia, I. She more than he: gender bias supports the empathic nature of yawn contagion in Homo sapiens. Open Sci. Difference in contagious yawning between susceptible men and women: why not?
Yawn contagion and empathy in Homo sapiens. Palagi, E. The neuroethology of spontaneous mimicry and emotional contagion in human and non-human animals. Contagious yawning in gelada baboons as a possible expression of empathy. Yawn contagion in humans and bonobos: emotional affinity matters more than species. Peer J. Preston, S.
Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases. Brain Sci. Provine, R. Yawning as a stereotyped action pattern and releasing stimulus. Ethology 72, — Faces as releasers of contagious yawning: an approach to face perception using normal human subjects. Heyes and B. Galef Jr. It was predicted that yawning would be less frequent in summer trials, but that it may be common just after entering the outside conditions since adaptive thermoregulatory responses take time to occur after encountering sudden climate changes.
However, the rate of yawning should diminish sharply as the amount of time spent outside increases, since it would no longer result in a cooling response. Contagious yawning was used as a proxy for yawning in both conditions because it is indistinguishable from spontaneous yawning aside from the fact that the triggers differ , and it can be manipulated using visual stimuli e.
In other words, a stretching of the jaw and a deep inhalation of air accompany both responses, and thus the physiological consequences should be similar. Furthermore, the study of contagious yawning provides the opportunity to determine whether underlying features related to thermal homeostasis mediate socially derived aspects of this behavior.
In total, 80 participants were recruited during each season winter: 38 males, 42 females; summer: 32 males, 48 females. All participants were over 18 years of age, gave verbal consent to participate in this study, and the Human Subjects Review Board at Binghamton University approved this research.
Pedestrians were approached by trained researchers while walking outside in public sidewalks and courtyards and asked to participate in a survey about contagious yawning. After viewing the images, participants self-reported on 1 whether they yawned during the experiment for validity of this approach see Greco and Baenninger, , 2 how many hours they slept the night before, and 3 how long they had been continuously outside before being approached to partake in this study.
One person in the winter condition and two individuals in the summer condition did not respond to the last question. Because research suggests that participants are less likely to yawn when they are being observed Baenninger and Greco, , the experimenters immediately walked away from the participants after they agreed to take the survey and intentionally avoided directing their attention toward them until it was completed.
While post-survey yawns were not used in the analysis, it is notable that four participants in the winter condition did not report yawning during the experiment but yawned while handing in the survey to the experimenter. Based on recent research showing no difference in contagious yawning rates between the hours of and Giganti and Zilli, , all participant recruitment occurred from to h.
Therefore any difference in the incidence of contagious yawning between seasons should not be result of this distinction. Statistical analyses were performed across and between conditions. Chi-square tests were used to test for differences in yawning frequency between seasonal climate conditions. Independent samples t -tests were performed to investigate differences between the two conditions in relative humidity, ambient temperature, time spent outside prior to testing, and sleep reported the night before.
Lastly, a binary logistic regression was used to explore which variables were associated with the tendency to yawn across and between conditions, with Nagelkerke R 2 being reported for significant results.
In total, 55 of the There were no sex differences in the response of social contagion, with 24 of 70 In other words, yawning occurred more frequently at lower ambient temperatures after controlling for other variables. Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics for all variables both within and across conditions.
During the summer when the climate was significantly warmer and dryer, pedestrians spent less time outdoors prior to testing. Figure 1. The proportion of participants yawning per seasonal climate condition. Similar binary logistic regression analyses were conducted separately for each seasonal condition. However, the summer condition showed the exact inverse relationship, indicating that the less time an individual was exposed to the climate conditions i.
As predicted from the thermal window hypothesis of thermoregulatory model, the incidence of contagious yawning in humans was influenced by seasonal variation in climate conditions. According to this hypothesis, it is the temperature of the ambient air that gives a yawn its cooling utility and thus it was expected that yawning should be diminished when outdoor temperatures reach or exceed body temperature.
Across seasonal trials yawning was associated with lower ambient temperatures, and in particular, yawning was less frequent in the summer condition when temperatures were higher and humidity was lower. Furthermore, the proportion of individuals yawning in the summer dropped greatly as the length of time spent outside increased, suggesting that the expression of social yawning may reflect a compromise with thermal effects.
On the other hand, there was a positive relationship between time spent outdoors prior to testing and yawning in the winter condition. Feelings of tiredness, boredom, and stress tend to make people yawn more. Excessive yawning may also stem from taking in too much caffeine or going through an opiate detox. You can also try taking a walk outside or finding a space with a cooler temperature.
Tell your doctor when the yawning started and about other symptoms, such as mind fog, pain in certain areas, or even lack of sleep.
This information can help your doctor diagnose the underlying condition and make treatment recommendations based on individual needs. There are many theories behind why we yawn. Read our tips on sleep hygiene for better quality sleep. Yawning is a natural response to being tired. Excessive yawning is yawning that occurs more than once per minute. Although excessive yawning is…. If your ears feel "full," you may find it helpful to pop them. In most cases, popping your ears is as easy as getting your mouth muscles moving….
Jaw popping can be caused by dysfunction of joints in the jaw. It's generally not a cause for concern if there's no pain. Despite all these theories, the truth is that scientists do not know the true biological function of a yawn. What we do know is that yawning occurs in just about every species. It happens when an animal is tired. It can be used as a threat display in some species. Yawning can occur during times of social conflict and stress, something researchers call a displacement behavior.
And that wide-open mouth can be contagious , especially in social species such as humans , chimpanzees , bonobos , macaques and wolves. Watching someone yawn—heck, even reading about yawns—can lead you to yawn yourself. Research on humans tell us that people who are more empathetic tend to be more susceptible to contagious yawning. When you see someone else yawn, the networks in your brain responsible for empathy and social skills are activated. Is yawning contagious for dogs, too?
0コメント